Objectives
By the end
of this text, you’ll be able to:
- Reflect on the key features and
stages involved when children acquire their first language (L1).
- Understand and analyze the
similarities and differences between acquiring a language naturally
(acquisition) and learning it consciously (learning).
- Compare how acquiring a first
language (L1) differs from acquiring or learning a second language (L2).
1. What
Is Competence in Language?
When we
talk about competence in language, we mean the internal knowledge
speakers have about their language—things like sounds (phonology), word forms
(morphology), vocabulary (lexicon), and meaning (semantics). Noam Chomsky, a
pioneering linguist, described this as “mental grammar” — the system your brain
naturally builds, without formal teaching.
Think about
walking: you don’t need lessons to learn to walk, right? You just do it.
The same applies to language. Babies aren’t taught language like they’re taught
math or history; they absorb it effortlessly (Chomsky, 1994). The truth
is, language acquisition is an innate ability.
But there’s
a difference between competence—what we know—and performance—how
we actually use language in real life. For example, if someone says, “We loosed
the car keys last weekend,” is this a mistake in their underlying knowledge of
English (competence), or just a slip of the tongue (performance)?
- A native speaker might
recognize it as a performance error—maybe they meant “lost.”
- A second language learner might
genuinely be unsure of the correct past tense verb.
Understanding
this difference helps us as teachers to respond appropriately in the classroom.
2. How
Do Children Acquire Their First Language (L1)?
Interestingly,
no matter where children grow up—across different cultures, socioeconomic
backgrounds, or education levels—they tend to acquire language in similar,
predictable stages. Most children reach full command of their mother tongue
around age five (Kess, 1993). But how do they manage such a complex task
without formal teaching?
Here’s a
simple breakdown of the main stages in first language acquisition:
Stage |
Age Range |
What Happens |
Pre-speech |
2–4 months |
Babies make sounds expressing comfort or
discomfort; “vocal play” begins |
Babbling |
6–8 months |
Repeated syllables, like “ba-ba” or “da-da” |
One-word / Holophrastic |
9–18 months |
Single words express entire ideas (e.g.,
“milk” could mean “I want milk”) |
Two-word stage |
18–24 months |
Simple two-word combinations (“mommy go,”
“doggie big”) |
Telegraphic / Early multi-word |
24–30 months |
Short sentences lacking function words (“want
juice,” “go store”) |
Later multi-word |
30+ months |
More complex sentences with grammar and
vocabulary expansion |
Imagine a
baby saying “milk.” They might mean “I want milk” or “There’s milk,” depending
on context. Sometimes they use a word too narrowly (underextension), or
too broadly (overextension). For example, a toddler calling all
four-legged animals “doggie.”
Babies
start with sounds they can easily produce and gradually build up to sentences,
much like learning to walk before running.
3. What
Helps Children Acquire Language?
Caregivers
naturally adjust how they speak to babies, a style often called motherese
or infant-directed speech. This speech is slow, uses short sentences,
higher pitch, and exaggerated vowels—all designed to help babies notice and
learn language patterns (Lightbrown & Spada, 2006).
For
instance, a mother might say, “Look at the biiig dog!” elongating the vowels to
highlight important sounds. This style captures a baby’s attention and helps
them differentiate sounds, recognize words, and even understand emotion through
intonation.
Research
confirms how crucial this input is:
- Babies exposed to repetitive
language at 7 months tend to have larger vocabularies at 24 months
(Newman, 2015).
- Clearer vowel sounds from
mothers predict better vocabulary growth between 15-19 months
(Kalasnikova, 2018).
- When mothers experience
postpartum depression and speak in lower, less expressive tones,
children’s language development suffers (Kaplan, 2012).
- Late talkers often receive less
infant-directed speech and lower pitch from caregivers (D'Odorico &
Jacob, 2006).
Babies are
social learners. They prefer this kind of speech and use feedback from
adults—who often repeat their attempts and expand on them—to build grammar and
vocabulary naturally.
4.
Cognitive Development and Language: How Are They Connected?
There’s an
ongoing debate in psycholinguistics about how much general cognitive
development influences language acquisition:
- Some say that children need
certain cognitive abilities to build language skills.
- Others argue that language
learning mechanisms are specialized and work independently.
- Noam Chomsky’s theory of Universal
Grammar suggests children are born with an innate understanding of
linguistic categories and rules common to all human languages.
While
acquiring a first language tends to happen naturally, learning a second
language usually requires conscious effort—like studying vocabulary and
practicing grammar. Adults learning an L2 often need explicit teaching,
repetition, and memorization, which children acquiring L1 don’t.
This leads
to important differences between L1 and L2 acquisition.
5.
Comparing First and Second Language Acquisition
What
explains the differences between L1 and L2 acquisition? Several factors come into play:
- Age: There’s a “critical period” for language
acquisition, roughly from birth to puberty (Lennenberg, 1967). Learning an
L2 after this period might mean you never reach native-like proficiency,
especially in pronunciation.
- Brain plasticity: Younger brains are more
flexible and better at picking up sounds and grammar.
- Learning environment: L1 happens naturally in a
rich, interactive home environment. L2 often happens in classrooms or
formal settings.
- Motivation and emotions: Children learning L1 are
driven by basic communication needs. Adults learning L2 may have different
motivations—work, travel, school—which affect how much they practice and
engage.
- Affective factors: Stress, anxiety, and
attitudes toward the language can either help or hinder learning.
For
example, a child growing up in a Spanish-speaking household will effortlessly
pick up Spanish. Meanwhile, an adult learning Spanish as a second language in a
classroom might struggle more with pronunciation and grammar.
6. The
Critical Period Hypothesis (CPH)
Lennenberg
(1967) proposed that there’s a critical period for language acquisition: if
children don’t learn their first language before puberty, their ability to
acquire it fully diminishes.
This idea
has been extended to second languages, but the evidence is mixed.
- In favor: Hartshorne, Tenenbaum, and
Pinker (2018) analyzed data from millions of English speakers and found
that grammar learning ability declines after about 17 years old.
- Against: Yusa et al. (2011) showed
that adults can still learn L2 with neural plasticity, suggesting some
aspects of language learning don’t have strict critical periods.
So, the
debate continues—and it’s important to consider individual differences.
7. Final
Thoughts and Practical Reflection
As
bilingual teacher trainees, understanding these stages and factors gives you
powerful insight into how children and adults acquire languages. This knowledge
will help you design better lessons, offer the right support, and appreciate
the amazing, natural capacity humans have for language.
Ask
yourself:
- Have you noticed students who
struggle more with pronunciation than grammar? Could age or motivation be
factors?
- How can you use motherese-like
strategies—simple, clear, repetitive speech—to support beginners in your
class?
- What role does emotion play in
your students’ language learning?
Remember,
language acquisition is a journey—sometimes smooth, sometimes challenging—but
always remarkable.
References
Chomsky, N.
(1994). The human language series.
Kess, D.
(1993). Language development and acquisition.
Lightbrown,
P. M., & Spada, N. (2006). How languages are learned (3rd ed.).
Oxford University Press.
Newman, R.
S. (2015). The role of repetition in early vocabulary acquisition. Journal
of Child Language, 42(4), 842-865.
Kalasnikova,
N. et al. (2018). Hyperarticulation and vocabulary growth in toddlers. Developmental
Science, 21(3).
Kaplan, P.
(2012). Maternal depression and infant language development. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(6), 585-594.
D'Odorico,
L., & Jacob, V. (2006). Infant-directed speech and late talkers. First
Language, 26(2), 149-163.
Lennenberg,
E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. Wiley.
Hartshorne,
J. K., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Pinker, S. (2018). A critical period for second
language acquisition. Cognition, 177, 263-277.
Yusa, N.,
Koizumi, M., Kim, J., et al. (2011). Neural plasticity in adult L2 learners. Journal
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 2716-2730.
No comments:
Post a Comment