To understand how we acquire, use, and teach language today, it's important to take a quick journey through the history of how language itself has been studied. You might be surprised to see how ideas have shifted—from simply listing "correct forms" to exploring how our body, culture, and mind shape the way we speak and understand each other.
Before
Modern Linguistics: Grammar and Philology
Long before
modern linguistics emerged, scholars were mostly focused on identifying what
was correct or incorrect in language. This was the job of traditional grammar.
Others
studied philology, which was broader. It looked at etymology
(word origins), grammar, rhetoric, and even literary criticism,
mainly focused on ancient Indo-European languages. Then came comparative
philology, which tried to compare different languages. But this had its
limits—it often ignored the deeper structures or purposes of language use in
real life.
Saussure
and the Birth of Modern Linguistics
The real
shift came with Ferdinand de Saussure, often called the "father of
modern linguistics." In his lectures—later published as Course in
General Linguistics—Saussure proposed that language isn't just about
individual words or grammar rules. It's about how we as a society structure and
make sense of the world through language.
He
introduced key ideas like:
- Langue vs. Parole: "Langue" is the
system of a language (like grammar), while "Parole" is how
people actually use it in daily conversation.
- Signifier and Signified: The word we use (like “tree”)
is the signifier; the concept we picture is the signified. Saussure argued
that the connection between the two is arbitrary—it's not based on
nature, but on social agreement.
- Synchronic vs. Diachronic study: Linguistics can focus on a
language at a specific moment (synchronic) or study its evolution through
time (diachronic).
However,
Saussure ignored the role of the human mind in producing language and
didn’t account for culture’s influence. His focus remained on the structure
of language, not its meaning or use.
What
Came After: Structuralism and Beyond
After
Saussure, structuralism became the dominant school of thought. It saw
language as a system made of parts that relate to each other, much like
a puzzle. While this helped create a more scientific study of language, it also
overemphasized structure and neglected the messy, emotional, and social
side of communication (Chandler, 2017).
Semiotics:
How Signs Make Meaning
Inspired by
Saussure, Charles Sanders Peirce expanded the idea of signs in what we
now call semiotics. He proposed three kinds of signs:
- Icon: Resembles what it represents
(e.g., a photo of a dog).
- Index: Has a direct link (e.g.,
smoke indicates fire).
- Symbol: Has no natural link, but we
agree on its meaning (e.g., traffic lights or letters in the alphabet).
Peirce
emphasized that signs only become meaningful when someone interprets them. In
other words, we are the meaning-makers (Peirce, 1960).
Functionalism:
Language in Action
Then came Roman
Jakobson, who focused on how language functions in real communication. He
described six elements (or “factors”) involved in every communication event:
the context, the sender, the receiver, the message,
the channel (contact), and the code (shared language).
Each
element corresponds to a function:
- Informative (giving facts)
- Emotive (expressing feelings)
- Imperative (giving commands)
- Poetic (using language artistically)
This
approach helps teachers understand that language is not just structure—it’s
also about intention, emotion, and interaction (Jakobson, 1981).
Halliday
and Language Acquisition
Michael
Halliday explored
how children acquire language naturally. Studying his own child, he found that
children don’t just learn words—they learn to use language to meet needs, make
friends, express emotions, and explore the world (Halliday, 1975).
This
challenged the idea that language is learned only by imitation (behaviourism)
or that it's entirely hardwired (nativism). Halliday saw language as a tool
for social interaction.
Chomsky
and Universal Grammar
In
contrast, Noam Chomsky revolutionized linguistics by focusing on syntax—the
structure of sentences. He believed language is innate, meaning we're
born with a kind of mental blueprint called universal grammar (Chomsky,
1965). According to him, our brains automatically generate grammatical
structures, no matter what language we speak.
However,
Chomsky also claimed that syntax is independent of meaning. For him,
language is a mental system, and the body or culture plays no essential role.
Critiques
of Chomsky: Meaning Matters
Later
thinkers like George Lakoff and Mark Johnson (1999) disagreed with
Chomsky. They argued that meaning, culture, and bodily experience deeply
influence how language is structured and used. After all, we speak with
emotions, gestures, and cultural references—not just with grammars.
They
proposed that syntax is shaped by how we want to express meaning, our
sensory experiences, and our cultural norms.
Cognitive
Linguistics: Bridging Mind, Body, and Culture
All of this
led to the development of Cognitive Linguistics, which says that
language is connected to:
- Our bodily experience
(e.g., pain, joy, movement),
- Our cognitive abilities
(thinking, remembering, imagining),
- And our social and cultural
world.
This
perspective helps us understand that language is not a separate system,
but a reflection of how we live, feel, and connect with others (Evans &
Green, 2006).
Connecting
to the Classroom
So, what
does this mean for you as a future bilingual teacher?
It means
that language is more than grammar drills or vocabulary lists. It’s a
living, breathing process that involves the mind, the body, and the community.
When you teach a language, you're helping students build connections between
meaning, culture, and expression.
You’re not
just teaching a code. You’re helping students interpret signs, engage
in social dialogue, and express themselves in a new world.
And the
truth is, that’s both a privilege and a powerful responsibility.
✅ References
Chandler,
D. (2017). Semiotics: The Basics (3rd ed.). Routledge.
Chomsky, N.
(1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. MIT Press.
Evans, V.,
& Green, M. (2006). Cognitive Linguistics: An Introduction. Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Halliday,
M. A. K. (1975). Learning How to Mean: Explorations in the Development of
Language. Edward Arnold.
Jakobson,
R. (1981). Selected Writings II: Word and Language. Mouton.
Lakoff, G.,
& Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and Its
Challenge to Western Thought. Basic Books.
Peirce, C.
S. (1960). Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce (Vols. 1–8).
Harvard University Press.
Sreedharan,
E. (2007). A Textbook of English Phonetics for Indian Students.
Macmillan.
No comments:
Post a Comment